Blind people remember language better than sighted people do

April 28, 2022

"Blind people may use language like a mental tool to remember information." They wondered if blind participants would outperform sighted ones at remembering spoken sounds. As the researchers expected, blind participants outperformed sighted ones on remembering speech. Blind participants again remembered more letters than sighted participants despite being forced to multitask mentally. "By using meaningless sound effects, we prevented participants from using language to remember them this lowered blind people's usual memory advantage" said Bedny.

Name
Jason Lucas
Email
[email protected]
Cell phone
443-301-7993

Blind people can remember speech better than sighted people, but a person's ability to see makes no difference in how they remember sound effects, found a new study by Johns Hopkins University and the University of California, Irvine.

"It's interesting that people who are blind only showed an advantage with verbal memory," said senior author Marina Bedny, an associate professor of psychology and brain sciences at Johns Hopkins whose work regularly compares blind and sighted individuals' brains. "Blind people may use language like a mental tool to remember information."

The findings appear in Experimental Brain Research.

Researchers conducted two memory tests with 20 blind adults and 22 blindfolded sighted adults. They wondered if blind participants would outperform sighted ones at remembering spoken sounds. First, participants listened to series of letters, followed by a delay. Then they heard either the same series or a 'foil' series where a letter is replaced or put into the wrong position. Participants then judged whether the second series of letters was the same as the first. For the second test, they listened to letters while solving mathematical equations with proposed answers. Participants determined if equations solutions were correct, followed by reciting back the letters.

Karen Arcos

Postdoctoral fellow, University of California, Santa Cruz

"[We think] the brain area responsible for vision in sighted people, the 'visual' cortex, is repurposed for other functions in blind people. Perhaps it enhances blind people's language processing."

As the researchers expected, blind participants outperformed sighted ones on remembering speech. The results from another testing phase, which required solving mathematical equations and recalling letters, confirmed researchers' predictions. Blind participants again remembered more letters than sighted participants despite being forced to multitask mentally.

"On a daily basis, blind people use their memory much more to remember things, while sighted people can rely on visual clues to recall information," said Karen Arcos, lead author and a blind postdoctoral fellow at University of California, Santa Cruz who earned her PhD at University of California, Irvine. "We think blind people's advantages on the verbal tests stem from increased practice remembering information. The brain area responsible for vision in sighted people, the 'visual' cortex, is repurposed for other functions in blind people. Perhaps it enhances blind people's language processing."

In another experimental phase, participants listened to two streams of sound effects and were asked if sounds matched. The researchers used sound effects such as tones and high-pitched beeps rather than everyday sounds to ensure sounds couldn't be labeled with words. On this task, blind and sighted people performed essentially the same.

"By using meaningless sound effects, we prevented participants from using language to remember them this lowered blind people's usual memory advantage" said Bedny.

Bedny is now studying what enables blind people to outperform sighted people at remembering words, letters, and numbers. Moreover, she plans to examine if the 'visual' cortex contributes to improved memory for speech and language in those born blind.

Additional authors include Nora Harhen, a graduate student at the University of California, Irvine; and Rita Loiotile, a former Johns Hopkins graduate student.

The research was supported by a Johns Hopkins Science of Learning Grant DGE-1321846, a grant from the National Eye Institute at the National Institutes of Health 1R01EY027352-01A, and the National Science Foundation Graduate Research and University of California President's Postdoctoral fellowships awarded to Arcos.

The source of this news is from Johns Hopkins University

Popular in Research

China is using the world's largest online disinformation operation to harass Americans

Nov 15, 2023

Child sexual abuse survivors lend their voice to support others

Nov 15, 2023

Print on demand business with Printseekers.com

Sep 6, 2022

Cost of living pressures sees social cohesion hit record low

Nov 15, 2023

Professor Emeritus Walter Hollister, an expert in flight instrumentation and guidance, dies at 92

Nov 15, 2023

Cool Course: City as Text

Nov 15, 2023